Tuesday, July 22, 2014

[L633.Ebook] Free PDF Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman

Free PDF Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman

You can carefully include the soft file Spending Time With The Lord, By Bill Freeman to the device or every computer hardware in your office or home. It will help you to constantly continue reading Spending Time With The Lord, By Bill Freeman every time you have leisure. This is why, reading this Spending Time With The Lord, By Bill Freeman does not give you issues. It will provide you important sources for you who wish to start creating, writing about the comparable publication Spending Time With The Lord, By Bill Freeman are various book industry.

Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman

Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman



Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman

Free PDF Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman

Book fans, when you require an extra book to read, find guide Spending Time With The Lord, By Bill Freeman right here. Never ever worry not to locate exactly what you need. Is the Spending Time With The Lord, By Bill Freeman your required book now? That's true; you are really an excellent visitor. This is an excellent book Spending Time With The Lord, By Bill Freeman that comes from excellent author to share with you. Guide Spending Time With The Lord, By Bill Freeman offers the best experience as well as lesson to take, not only take, but also learn.

There is no doubt that book Spending Time With The Lord, By Bill Freeman will still give you inspirations. Also this is merely a book Spending Time With The Lord, By Bill Freeman; you could locate several genres and sorts of books. From entertaining to experience to politic, and also scientific researches are all provided. As just what we mention, right here we offer those all, from well-known writers and publisher in the world. This Spending Time With The Lord, By Bill Freeman is one of the compilations. Are you interested? Take it now. Just how is the means? Learn more this article!

When someone needs to visit the book stores, search store by establishment, shelf by rack, it is quite troublesome. This is why we supply guide collections in this web site. It will certainly ease you to search guide Spending Time With The Lord, By Bill Freeman as you such as. By browsing the title, author, or writers of guide you want, you can locate them promptly. In your home, office, or perhaps in your method can be all ideal area within net links. If you want to download and install the Spending Time With The Lord, By Bill Freeman, it is very easy then, considering that now we extend the connect to buy as well as make bargains to download and install Spending Time With The Lord, By Bill Freeman So simple!

Curious? Of course, this is why, we suppose you to click the link web page to go to, then you can delight in the book Spending Time With The Lord, By Bill Freeman downloaded until completed. You could save the soft file of this Spending Time With The Lord, By Bill Freeman in your device. Naturally, you will bring the device almost everywhere, won't you? This is why, whenever you have downtime, every time you can enjoy reading by soft copy publication Spending Time With The Lord, By Bill Freeman

Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman

  • Sales Rank: #891399 in Books
  • Published on: 1999-11-03
  • Original language: English
  • Number of items: 1
  • Dimensions: .59" h x 5.28" w x 8.04" l,
  • Binding: Paperback
  • 223 pages

About the Author
Bill Freeman has ministered the Word for over forty years in California, Arizona, and Washington. He is the editor of a periodical publication called The Christian, and founder of Ministry of the Word, Inc., a ministry serving the Body of Christ through publications, the Internet, and conferences. Bill is a graduate of Fuller Theological Seminary and has written over forty books, booklets, and outline studies on the believer's experience of Christ in daily life and its practical relationship to the church.

Most helpful customer reviews

4 of 6 people found the following review helpful.
A Wonderful Book
By Kindle Customer
This is a wonderful book that warms your heart and makes you want to spend time with Jesus each day. It also makes you value the importance of spending time Him.

See all 3 customer reviews...

Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman PDF
Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman EPub
Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman Doc
Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman iBooks
Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman rtf
Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman Mobipocket
Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman Kindle

Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman PDF

Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman PDF

Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman PDF
Spending Time with the Lord, by Bill Freeman PDF

Wednesday, July 16, 2014

[K585.Ebook] PDF Download Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz

PDF Download Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz

Sport Management: The Basics, By Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz. Allow's read! We will usually discover this sentence everywhere. When still being a children, mother utilized to get us to always read, so did the instructor. Some books Sport Management: The Basics, By Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz are completely reviewed in a week and we require the commitment to support reading Sport Management: The Basics, By Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz What about now? Do you still like reading? Is reviewing only for you which have commitment? Definitely not! We below offer you a new e-book qualified Sport Management: The Basics, By Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz to check out.

Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz

Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz



Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz

PDF Download Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz

Sport Management: The Basics, By Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz. What are you doing when having leisure? Talking or surfing? Why do not you aim to review some e-book? Why should be reading? Checking out is one of enjoyable and enjoyable activity to do in your downtime. By checking out from lots of sources, you can discover new info and also encounter. Guides Sport Management: The Basics, By Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz to read will certainly many beginning from scientific publications to the fiction books. It implies that you could read guides based on the need that you wish to take. Certainly, it will be various as well as you can review all book types at any time. As here, we will reveal you a publication must be checked out. This e-book Sport Management: The Basics, By Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz is the selection.

When getting this book Sport Management: The Basics, By Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz as recommendation to check out, you can acquire not just motivation however additionally new understanding and sessions. It has greater than usual benefits to take. What type of book that you read it will work for you? So, why must obtain this book qualified Sport Management: The Basics, By Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz in this write-up? As in link download, you could get guide Sport Management: The Basics, By Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz by on the internet.

When getting the e-book Sport Management: The Basics, By Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz by on the internet, you could read them any place you are. Yeah, even you remain in the train, bus, hesitating list, or other areas, on-line e-book Sport Management: The Basics, By Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz could be your good pal. Every single time is an excellent time to read. It will certainly enhance your knowledge, fun, amusing, session, as well as experience without investing more money. This is why on-line book Sport Management: The Basics, By Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz comes to be most wanted.

Be the initial which are reading this Sport Management: The Basics, By Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz Based upon some factors, reading this e-book will certainly supply more perks. Also you have to read it detailed, page by page, you could finish it whenever as well as any place you have time. Once again, this online e-book Sport Management: The Basics, By Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz will certainly offer you simple of checking out time and task. It likewise provides the experience that is affordable to get to and also obtain greatly for far better life.

Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz

Sport Management: The Basics is an engaging and accessible introduction to sport management which considers a range of contemporary philosophical, social, cultural and political matters as they impact on this growing field. Drawing links between academic theory and practice, it explores the current challenges facing managers in the sport industry, addressing topics including:

  • the history of sport management
  • the role of the manager
  • levels of management
  • the public, private and voluntary sectors
  • sport management in�the global marketplace

With suggestions for further reading throughout the text, a comprehensive chapter on employment and employability, and case studies which explore both theory and practice, Sport Management: The Basics offers a clear and concise introduction for anyone seeking to study or work in sport management.

  • Sales Rank: #1457848 in Books
  • Published on: 2015-10-08
  • Original language: English
  • Number of items: 1
  • Dimensions: 7.70" h x .50" w x 5.10" l, .0 pounds
  • Binding: Paperback
  • 212 pages

Review

‘Those interested in the sport management profession will appreciate the case studies and excellent examples, which not only artfully illustrate the theory and practice of sport administration but also offer genuine insights into the industry’s career and leadership opportunities. Summing Up: Recommended’ – S. R. Kahn, University of Cincinnati, CHOICE

About the Author

Rob Wilson is Principal Lecturer in Sport Business Management at Sheffield Hallam University, UK. He�has delivered sport management-related courses since 2002 and his subject specialisms are financial reporting, management accounting and economic decision making in the sport industry. He is the author of Managing Sport Finance, the co-author of Finance for Sport and Leisure Managers: An introduction and has published a number of chapters in sport management text books.�He is also a Senior Fellow of the Higher Education Academy and a recognised expert in learning and teaching in higher education.

Mark Piekarz is Senior Lecturer in Sport Management at the University of Worcester, UK. He has more than 25 years of experience in the sport management and adventure sport sectors. Before beginning his teaching work in Higher Education he worked for many years as a sport facility manager and community sport development officer. He has since taught at the Birmingham College of Food and Technology, UK, and Sheffield Hallam University,UK, before completing a PhD in the field of risk management. He has published work on a variety of subject areas, ranging from risk management, sport and rights, conflict zone tourism and operational management

Most helpful customer reviews

See all customer reviews...

Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz PDF
Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz EPub
Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz Doc
Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz iBooks
Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz rtf
Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz Mobipocket
Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz Kindle

Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz PDF

Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz PDF

Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz PDF
Sport Management: The Basics, by Rob Wilson, Mark Piekarz PDF

Sunday, July 6, 2014

[T712.Ebook] Download Ebook The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk

Download Ebook The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk

From currently, discovering the finished website that sells the completed publications will be many, yet we are the relied on site to check out. The Genius In All Of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, And IQ, By David Shenk with easy web link, simple download, and also completed book collections become our great solutions to get. You can discover and make use of the advantages of picking this The Genius In All Of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, And IQ, By David Shenk as every little thing you do. Life is constantly creating and you need some brand-new book The Genius In All Of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, And IQ, By David Shenk to be reference consistently.

The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk

The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk



The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk

Download Ebook The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk

How if there is a website that allows you to hunt for referred book The Genius In All Of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, And IQ, By David Shenk from throughout the globe publisher? Immediately, the site will be amazing finished. Numerous book collections can be found. All will be so easy without complex thing to move from website to website to obtain the book The Genius In All Of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, And IQ, By David Shenk wanted. This is the site that will offer you those expectations. By following this site you can acquire whole lots varieties of publication The Genius In All Of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, And IQ, By David Shenk collections from versions types of author and also author prominent in this world. Guide such as The Genius In All Of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, And IQ, By David Shenk and also others can be gotten by clicking wonderful on web link download.

However right here, we will show you amazing point to be able always check out the e-book The Genius In All Of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, And IQ, By David Shenk anywhere and also whenever you take place as well as time. The publication The Genius In All Of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, And IQ, By David Shenk by just could help you to realize having the book to check out every time. It will not obligate you to always bring the thick book anywhere you go. You can merely maintain them on the kitchen appliance or on soft data in your computer system to constantly read the space at that time.

Yeah, hanging around to review the publication The Genius In All Of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, And IQ, By David Shenk by on-line can also give you positive session. It will certainly alleviate to talk in whatever problem. By doing this could be much more interesting to do and also much easier to read. Now, to get this The Genius In All Of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, And IQ, By David Shenk, you can download and install in the web link that we provide. It will help you to obtain easy method to download and install guide The Genius In All Of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, And IQ, By David Shenk.

The e-books The Genius In All Of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, And IQ, By David Shenk, from basic to challenging one will be a quite helpful operates that you can take to change your life. It will not provide you negative statement unless you do not get the significance. This is definitely to do in checking out a book to get rid of the significance. Frequently, this e-book qualified The Genius In All Of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, And IQ, By David Shenk is read because you really such as this type of book. So, you could obtain easier to comprehend the impression and also definition. As soon as longer to consistently remember is by reading this e-book The Genius In All Of Us: New Insights Into Genetics, Talent, And IQ, By David Shenk, you could satisfy hat your curiosity beginning by completing this reading e-book.

The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk

With irresistibly persuasive vigor, David Shenk debunks the long-standing notion of genetic “giftedness,” and presents dazzling new scientific research showing how greatness is in the reach of every individual.


DNA does not make us who we are. “Forget everything you think you know about genes, talent, and intelligence,” he writes. “In recent years, a mountain of scientific evidence has emerged suggesting a completely new paradigm: not talent scarcity, but latent talent abundance.”


Integrating cutting-edge research from a wide swath of disciplines—cognitive science, genetics, biology, child development—Shenk offers a highly optimistic new view of human potential. The problem isn't our inadequate genetic assets, but our inability, so far, to tap into what we already have. IQ testing and widespread acceptance of “innate” abilities have created an unnecessarily pessimistic view of humanity—and fostered much misdirected public policy, especially in education.


The truth is much more exciting. Genes are not a “blueprint” that bless some with greatness and doom most of us to mediocrity or worse. Rather our individual destinies are a product of the complex interplay between genes and outside stimuli-a dynamic that we, as people and as parents, can influence.


This is a revolutionary and optimistic message. We are not prisoners of our DNA. We all have the potential for greatness.

  • Sales Rank: #356166 in Books
  • Published on: 2011-03-08
  • Released on: 2011-03-08
  • Original language: English
  • Number of items: 1
  • Dimensions: 8.00" h x .90" w x 5.20" l, .82 pounds
  • Binding: Paperback
  • 400 pages

Amazon.com Review
Louann Brizendine Reviews The Genius In All of Us

Louann Brizendine, M.D.,author of The Female Brain and The Male Brain, is a diplomat of the American Board of Psychiatry and Neurology and the National Board of Medical Examiners, and is clinical professor of psychiatry at UCSF. She is founder and director of the Women's Mood and Hormone Clinic and the Teen Girl Mood and Hormone Clinic. After receiving her medical degree from Yale University School of Medicine in New Haven, Connecticut, she completed an internship in medicine and neurology at Harvard Medical School's Brigham and Women's Hospital, and a residency in psychiatry at the Massachusetts Mental Health Center of Harvard Medical School. She sits on the boards of many prestigious peer reviewed journals and is the recipient of numerous honors and awards. Read Brizendine's guest review of The Genius In All of Us:

In The Genius in All of Us Shenk beautifully explains why the nature-nurture debate is dead. It is not just the genes we are born with, but how we are raised and what opportunities are open to us that determine how smart we will become. Nurture and experience reshape our genes, and thus our brain. Shenk argues that the idea we are either born with genius or talent, or we aren’t, is simply untrue. The notion that relentless, deliberate practice changes the brain and thus our abilities has been undervalued over the past 30 years in favor of the concept of “innate giftedness.” Practice, practice, practice (some say 10,000 hours or more) is what it takes. Shenk argues that it is just some fantasy that effortless, gifted genius is born and not made. He marshals evidence to show that genetic factors do not trump environmental factors but rather work in concert with them. Shenk notes that by the sweat of our brow we can train ourselves to be successful--even if we are born with only average genetic talent. Scientists know that how we are raised and how we are trained affects the expression of our genes. If you think you’ve reached your talent limit, think again, Shenk says. It’s not just in your genes, he says, but in the intensity of your motivation. Ambition, persistence, and self-discipline are not just products of genes, but can be shaped by nurture and environment. Certainly it is important to have good genes, but that determines at most only 50 percent of your talent. He underscores the point that intelligence is made up of the skills that a person has developed--with an emphasis on “developed”--through hard work. Encouraging ourselves and our children to work hard requires being surrounded by others also wanting to achieve striving for excellence. Moreover, Shenk gives the hopeful message not just for kids, but also for adults. Happily for us, the human brain remains plastic, changeable and trainable well into old age. So no matter how old you are, if you’d like to be smarter--get to work! --Louann Brizendine

A Q&A with David Shenk

Question: Your book is called The Genius in All of Us: Why Everything You’ve Been Told About Genetics, Talent, and IQ is Wrong. That’s a big claim. Everything and how so?

David Shenk: It is a bold statement, and it reflects how poorly the public has been served when it comes to understanding the relationship between biology and ability. The clich�s we’ve been taught about genetic blueprints, IQ, and "giftedness" all come out of crude, early-20th century guesswork. The reality is so much more interesting and complex. Genes do have a powerful influence on everything we do, but they respond to their environments in all sorts of interesting ways. We’ve now learned a lot more about the developmental mechanisms that enable people to get really good at stuff. Intelligence and talent turn out to be about process, not about whether you were born with certain "gifts."

Question: In The Genius in All of Us you state that the concept of nature versus nurture is over. Scientists, cognitive psychologists, and geneticists are moving towards an idea of ‘interactionism.' What does this mean? If the battle of genes versus environment is over, who has won? Which is more important?

David Shenk: They both won, because they're both vitally important. But the new science shows us that they do not act separately. Declaring that a person gets X-percent of his/her intelligence from genes and Y-percent from the environment is like saying that X-percent of Shakespeare's greatness can be found in his verbs, and Y-percent in his adjectives. There is no nature vs. nurture, or nature plus nurture; instead, it's nature interacting with nurture, which is often expressed by scientists as "GxE" (genes times intelligence). This is what "interactionism" refers to. A vanguard of geneticists, neuroscientists, and psychologists have stepped forward in recent years to articulate the importance of the dynamic interaction between genes and the environment.

Question: You describe genes and environment as a sound board. How so?

David Shenk: In the past, we’ve been taught that each distinct gene contains a certain dossier of information, which in turn determines a certain trait; if you have the blue-eyed gene, you get blue eyes. Period.

It turns out, though, that the information contained inside genes is only part of the story; another critical part is how often genes get "expressed," or turned on, by other genes and by outside forces. It’s therefore helpful to think of your genome as a giant mixing board with thousands of knobs and switches. Genes are always getting turned on/off/up/down by hormones, nutrients, etc. People actually affect their own genome’s behavior with their actions.

Question: How do these new findings affect the concept of the "The Bell Curve"--that we live in an increasingly stratified world where the "cognitive elite," those with the best genes, are more and more isolated from the cognitive/genetic underclass? Is that idea now completely obsolete?

David Shenk: Yes, it is obsolete. The idea that there is a genetic super-class that has a corner on high-IQ genes is nonsense. This comes out of a profound misunderstanding of how genes work and how intelligence works, and also from a misreading of so-called "heritability" studies. I am not saying that genes don’t affect intelligence. Genes affect everything. But by and large I think the evidence shows that people with low intelligence are missing out on key developmental advantages.

Question: Lewis Terman invented the IQ test at Stanford University in 1916. He declared it the ideal tool to determine a person’s native intelligence. Are IQ tests accurate? What are the benefits and fallout of the IQ test?

David Shenk: IQ tests accurately rank academic achievement. That’s quite different from identifying innate intelligence, which doesn’t really exist. Tufts intelligence expert Robert Sternberg explains that "intelligence represents a set of competencies in development." In other words, intelligence isn’t fixed. Intelligence isn’t general. Intelligence is not a thing. Instead, intelligence is a dynamic, diffuse, and ongoing process.

The IQ test has valid uses. It can help teachers and principals understand how well students are doing and what they’re missing. But the widespread belief that it defines what each of us are capable of (and limited to) is disabling for individuals and society. People simply cannot reach their full potential if they honestly believe that they are so severely restricted.

Question: How do we go about finding the genius in all of us? What steps we can take to unlock latent talent?

David Shenk: Find the thing you love to do, and work and work and work at it. Don't be discouraged by failure; realize that high achievers thrive on failure as a motivating mechanism and as instruction guide on how to get better.

(Photo � Alexandra Beers)

From Booklist
*Starred Review* Intent on burying the concept of inborn genius, Shenk uses the 128 pages of “The Argument” to drive home how mistaken the notion of being genetically destined at birth to be a Mozart or a Michael Jordan is. For genes aren’t the inalterable essences that environmental effects merely help out; rather, genes and environment interact to realize a person’s potential in a constant and complicated process that Shenk attractively exemplifies through the athletic life of Ted Williams, who began hitting practice at age six and, equally important, never gave it up; also, considered to have magically sharp sight, he tested only high normal upon entering naval aviation—the thing was, he developed himself to, as Ty Cobb said, “see more of the ball than any man alive.” En route to the startling revelation that Lamarckism (variation by inheritance, not Darwinian natural selection) has truth in it, after all, Shenk corrects common knowledge about what twin studies and IQ tests really show; clarifies the arguably most misunderstood genetics term, heritable; and scientifically revives faith in not just practice and determination but also parenting and lifestyle as crucial factors, along with genes, in the realization of talents. Since this flies in the face of a century of genetic determinism, Shenk employs the equally engrossing 141 pages of “The Evidence” to cite, quote, paraphrase, and comment upon the sources for virtually every assertion in “The Argument.” --Ray Olson

Review
"[Shenk] tells engaging stories, lucidly explains complex research and offers fresh insights in the nature of exceptional peformance,,,,such efforts have resulted in a deeply interesting and important book. David Shenk may not be a genius yet, but give him time."-- New York Times Book Review


"Inspired...The Genius in All of Us has quietly blown my mind."--Salon


“…a welcome new book…you’ll find [Shenk] a fluid, easy writer…The Genius in All of Us is a quick, compelling read.”
– The Boston Globe


"Empowering...myth-busting...highly readable...entertaining."�
-- KIRKUS REVIEWS��

"Intent on burying the concept of inborn genius, Shenk uses the 128 pages of "The Argument" to drive home how mistaken the notion of being genetically destined at birth to be a Mozart or a Michael Jordan is. For genes aren’t the inalterable essences that environmental effects merely help out; rather, genes and environment interact to realize a person’s potential in a constant and complicated process that Shenk attractively exemplifies through the athletic life of Ted Williams, who began hitting practice at age six and, equally important, never gave it up; also, considered to have magically sharp sight, he tested only high normal upon entering naval aviation—the thing was, he developed himself to, as Ty Cobb said, "see more of the ball than any man alive." En route to the startling revelation that Lamarckism (variation by inheritance, not Darwinian natural selection) has truth in it, after all, Shenk corrects common knowledge about what twin studies and IQ tests really show; clarifies the arguably most misunderstood genetics term, heritable; and scientifically revives faith in not just practice and determination but also parenting and lifestyle as crucial factors, along with genes, in the realization of talents. Since this flies in the face of a century of genetic determinism, Shenk employs the equally engrossing 141 pages of "The Evidence" to cite, quote, paraphrase, and comment upon the sources for virtually every assertion in "The Argument."�
--Booklist, starred review


"An inspiring and liberating book. It's a powerful antidote to the genetic determinism rampant in the Age of the Genome, and an instructive guide, grounded in science, to living a more enriching life."�
--STEVEN JOHNSON, author of�The Invention of Air, Ghost Map, Everything Bad is Good for You, Mind Wide Open, Emergence, and�Interface Culture


"This book, both rigorous and accessible, is a close study of the idea of genius, an investigation of popular misconceptions about genetics, and an examination of the American virtue of self-determination. �It is written with assurance, insight, clarity, and wit." --�ANDREW SOLOMON, author of�The Noonday Demon�(National Book Award Winner, 2001)


"A great book. David Shenk handily dispels the myth that one must be born a genius. From consistently whacking the ball out of the park to composing ethereal piano sonatas, Shenk convincingly makes the case for the potential genius that lies in all of us." -- RUDOLPH E. TANZI, PH.D., Joseph P. and Rose F. Kennedy�Professor of Neurology, Harvard Medical School; Director, Genetics and Aging Research Unit, Massachusetts General Hospital


"David Shenk sweeps aside decades of misconceptions about genetics -- and shows that by overstating the importance of genes, we've understated the potential of ourselves.�This is a persuasive and inspiring book that will make you think anew about your own life and our shared future." -- DANIEL H. PINK, author of�Drive�and�A Whole New Mind


"In clear, forceful language, backed up by a boatload of science, David Shenk delivers a message that should be read by every parent, educator, and policy-maker who cares about the future of our children. The Genius in All of Us convincingly debunks the "genes are destiny" argument when it comes to human talent, and will force you to rethink everything from IQ tests and twins studies to child-rearing practices. Shenk's book turns Baby Mozart on his head, and will give pause--a hopeful, empowering pause--to parents who wish to nurture excellence in their children." � ��
-- STEPHEN S. HALL, author of Wisdom: From Philosophy to Neuroscience


"David Shenk freshens and transforms a familiar subject to reveal all the interacting forces and factors that make us who we are. Development is a tricky business and Shenk understands this. By focusing his considerable writing talents on this terribly important topic, he has provided parents, policymakers, and educators with a book that will help them cut through the noise and make sense of every child's development."
-- MARK. S. BLUMBERG, Ph.D., F. Wendell Miller Professor of Psychology,�University�of�Iowa; editor-in-chief,�Behavioral Neuroscience;�and author,�Freaks of Nature: What Anomalies Tell Us about Development and Evolution


"The importance of David Shenk's book is that he has made accessible to a wide audience the advances in the understanding of how each person develops. I congratulate him."�-- SIR PATRICK BATESON,�FRS, Emeritus Professor of Ethology, Cambridge University; former Biological Secretary of the Royal Society (UK); and co-author,�Design For A Life: How Behaviour Develops�

Most helpful customer reviews

85 of 98 people found the following review helpful.
Well written, with a vital message
By Marcus Anthony
"This book is not a dispassionate presentation of all scientific points of view. Instead it embraces the arguments of the Interactionists, whose views I came to trust most after much reading, conversation and consideration."(p. 148)

So writes David Shenk in The Genius in All of Us, and true to his word he is. Shenk's book is not a strictly scientific investigation of intelligence or giftedness, but a personal presentation for the case that intelligence is highly malleable, and that it emerges from the interaction of genes and environment. His case differs from many mainstream representations of intelligence in that he finds environment plays a far greater role than many intelligence theorists acknowledge. Intelligence, states Shenk, is a process, more so than a discrete entity which sits in the physical structure of the brain. He writes:

"...intelligence isn't fixed. Intelligence isn't general. Intelligence is not a thing. Intelligence is a dynamic, diffuse and ongoing process." (p.42)

So, David Shenk does not even attempt to be even-handed, and barely addresses the criticisms to the interactionist position. Some readers won't like the book for that reason.

I had no problem with reading the book. There is no law which says that a non-fiction book has to take a critical approach to its own thesis. If you are looking for a look at the arguments from multiple perspectives, this is not the book for you. You might instead try Howard Gardner's Intelligence: Multiple Perspectives, or Ken Richardson's brilliant little book, The Making of Intelligence (though both books largely comply with Shenk's position). These are very readable and concise volumes.

Personally, I liked The Genius in Us All a great deal, and no doubt this reflects the fact that I agree with Shenk's essential argument. I have spent many years investigating cognitive development, including researching and practicing "learning how to learn", accelerated learning, intelligence theory, and neural plasticity. On top of this I have explored other ways of knowing, including the meditative and intuitive. Many years ago my older brother (who just happened to nick-name me "Dope" as a child - children are cruel!) was diagnosed with some mental problems. He was given an intelligence test. His IQ attribution was about half of the score I have been assigned in such tests, and it struck me that genetics are probably a poor explanation for the very different adults we had turned out to be. As a child, my brother had no interest in the scholastic at all, while I was obsessed with reading and writing. Later I developed myself further through years of mental work. I have come to strongly believe that mental ability is far more malleable than what is popularly depicted in mainstream science and the mass media. Given this, I am very sympathetic to Shenk's argument.

The Genius in All of Us is well written. Shenk's writing style is highly lucid, and he embellishes his ideas with interesting examples from real-life case studies. The book is divided into two sections. The first is some 130 pages of easy-to-read prose, which outlines Shenk's thesis, without the interruption of excessive references and quotations. These he leaves for the second half of the book. Those who wish to follow up with a more critical investigation of the subject, can proceed to this section after completing the first. Personally, I found the second half just as fascinating, but many may wish to conclude with the first. In short, The Genius is an excellent layman's account of the argument for the plasticity of intelligence. Shenk's enthusiasm for the subject shines through on all pages, and so I give it five stars. It achieves exactly what it sets out to do.

David Shenk believes that the concept of "g" (IQ), may have developed from the fact that western education teaches the very abilities that IQ tests test for. The entire IQ game then become a self-fulfilling prophecy, as those who work hard and succeed in the education system then test higher in IQ assessment. An interesting aside, although Shenk does not state this himself, is that the success of Asians in IQ tests can probably be attributed (in part) to the endless schooling and tests they do in their exceedingly rote-centred education systems (I have taught in Asia for ten years at all levels of the system). In this sense, IQ tests may reflect academic achievement as much as innate intelligence.

Shenk does not dismiss genetics and natural ability altogether as contributing to human intelligence. He simply states that their contribution is overstated. Something as complex as intelligence cannot be attributed, reductionist style, to the micro-processes within cells. Shenk quotes Cambridge university psychologist Patrick Bateson as saying that genes:

"...store information coding for the amino acid sequences of proteins... That is all. They do not code for parts of the nervous system and they certainly do not code for particular behavior patterns." (p. 21)

Hard work, discipline and self-sacrifice lie at the heart of many a story of "genius", Shenk finds. Amongst many examples, the author refers to the biography of Mozart, whose life is often cited as an example of innate giftedness. In fact, according to Shenk, Mozart was exposed to an extraordinarily stimulating musical environment almost from the moment he was born; and thus his remarkable achievements were at least in part a function of the environment in which he was raised, not to mention his extreme dedication to his chosen profession.

A related point raised by Shenk is that some evidence suggests children diagnosed as "gifted" rarely go on to be adult creators of note. Citing research from Stanford psychologist Carol Dweck, Shenk argues that a belief that intelligence is innate, rather than an interactive process, actually retards the full development of ability. Those with an interactionist perspective of intelligence tend to be far more intellectually ambitious and successful. (p. 235). It may be an irony that thinking you are born smart makes you dumb - well, dumber than you might otherwise have been. Further, child prodigies can become "frozen into expertise", and turn into risk avoiders. In short, their capacity for innovation is reduced by attitudes which emerge from their childhood self-concept and worldview. The reverse side of this coin is where people believe that they have an innate lack of intelligence and ability, and don't even try to express their potential capacities.

One recurring reference in The Genius is that of the Flynn effect - the fact that IQ scores are increasing about three points per generation. A fascinating statistic is that ninety-eight per cent of IQ test takers do better today than their counterparts did in 1900. Shenk rightly points out that if intelligence is purely genetic, IQ scores should remain stable from generation to generation. Clearly then, there is something important happening here. Shenk puts it down to the demands of modern education, and the fact the industrial and information society demand the development of a greater cognitive complexity, especially in the workplace. Increased leisure time may be another factor, including the influence of radio, television and the internet placing increasing demands on the intellect. Shenk also argues that dominant concepts within society help facilitate the expansion of intelligence. Recent centuries have seen the emergence of the idea of evolution, and the development of abstract thought, and the deep questioning of information. Flynn himself described the generations expansion of intelligence as a "cultural transition from pre-scientific to post-scientific thinking", and that it represented "nothing less than the liberation of the human mind." (p.36) These culturally mediated shifts in thinking are deeply ingrained in western populations today, but this was not always the case.

Another strong theme in the book is that genes do not encode for intelligence, and that the function of genetics in intelligence theory has been overstated (and in many other domains of inquiry). This oversell has occurred both in scientific and popular circles.

Personally, I would go even further than Shenk in the evaluating the implications of intelligence as malleable process.

Shenk states that the developmental paradigm "will... require not just a new intellectual leap, but also a moral, psychological, and spiritual leap." (p. 95) He cites biological, economic, cultural, nutritional, parental, and ecological" influences on cognitive development." (p.95). Quoting neuroscientists Mark H. Johnson and Annette Karmiloff-Smith, Shenk finds that cognitive development "is an activity-dependent process at the molecular, cellular, and organismal levels involving probabilistic epigenisis (biodirectional relatio0ns between genes, brain and behavior)." (p. 106)

Shenk is right to point out (citing McGill's Michael Meaney) that the intracellular environment (within cells) emerges from the genetic makeup of the cell and the extracellular environment (e.g. hormones, the immune system, neurotransmitters and nutrients), and that these in turn are influenced by the individual environment (p. 159). Neurotransmitter and hormonal activity, for example, are influenced by social interactions.

Yet beyond all these regulators of cognitive development there is the domain of "motivation", and this is what really fascinates me. Shenk addresses human intention briefly, but does not explore it depth, preferring to leave it as a kind of mystery. The author quotes Ellen Winner as saying that creators have a desire to shake things up, and are restless, rebellious, and dissatisfied with the status quo. They are "courageous and independent" (p. 226) But where do such qualities come from? It is here that mainstream science is at its weakest, and quite surprisingly, Shenk also.

At this juncture we begin to address the intangible, qualitative domains of consciousness, and ultimately the "spiritual". What is it that drives a person to dedicate the "magic" figure of 10 000 hours to develop his/her genius to a level of mastery?

Shenk has stretched the dominant paradigm -but I believe that it will be stretched even more in the coming decades and centuries, and that eventually it will "bust". Constricted by an overly reductionist model of mind and biology, Shenk stops enticingly short of expanding into the real frontiers of human intelligence - consciousness itself. He states that "any individual gene or environmental event produces an effect only by interacting with other genes and environments." (160, my italics) In other words intelligence is only ever gene or environment mediated. This is an unnecessary delimitation.
For example, he writes:

If genes are merely the bricklayers, where's the foreman. Where's the architect?
Amazingly, there's no architect. Like ant colonies, galaxies, and other complex emergent systems, the human body is a dynamic assembly abiding by certain strict laws of science but not following any master set of instructions. The outcome is a function of the ingredients and the process. (p. 159)

It appears that Shenk sees ontogeny (individual development) as an essentially random process, parroting the very worldview of the mechanistic views of mind and body that he seeks to discredit. This is an almost inexplicable contradiction to Shenk's a central tenet of his main thesis - that motivation is a prime driver in the development of intelligence and genius.

The next great step in the examination of intelligence will incorporate an expansion of the understanding of consciousness itself (though this may be a long way off). At one level, for example, we have human intention - the myriad of thoughts and emotional energies that run through the mind, generated both consciously and unconsciously by the individual. Thinking affects physiology, and in turn, the expression of genetic potential. So, first-person thought, or human intention will have to be added to the genetic and environmental mediators of intelligence that Shenk refers to . This is just common sense, and a surprising omission from Shenk's thesis.

Yet this is only the tip of the iceberg. Increasing evidence points to the fact that consciousness, like intelligence, is not a discrete entity which exists in finite brains, but is a system of information (much as I hate the word, I cannot think of a better one) which transcends the brain. There is no physicalist framework which adequately accommodates this expression of mind. Ultimately we will need to incorporate the concept of consciousness fields.

It won't be long before the whole book has to be re-written. In just one recent paper published in Nature (463, 644-647 -4 February 2010). Elisabetta Collini and other scientists presented evidence " that long-range quantum coherence between molecules can... be sustained in complex biological systems." The evidence is only going to become greater, and it won't be confined to cellular biology. Once it is established that "environment" incorporates consciousness fields that extend (theoretically) into infinite space (and beyond), the entire field of cognitive development, indeed cognitive science, will have to expand massively beyond its currently narrow confines. Once that happens, David Shenk's thesis will not appear so outrageous - in fact it will appear relatively conservative.

Flynn saw the development of scientific thinking as liberation from pre-scientific thinking. But the liberation is not yet complete. This recent "critical rationality" (as I call it) will eventually be enhanced by an intelligence which embraces the transrational intellect, and which incorporates brain-transcendent cognition - or what I call "integrated intelligence".

Still, one cannot blame David Shenk for simply "nibbling" at the periphery of the frontiers of human intelligence. As futurist John Naisbitt has commented, if you get too far ahead of the parade, nobody will be able to see you. No doubt I've lost a few readers since the beginning of this review!

David Shenk is correct. A whole new definition of intelligence will soon be required, and he has done a noble job in getting the ball rolling.

Marcus T. Anthony (PhD), author of the novel "The Mind Reader"; and "Extraordinary Mind"

94 of 110 people found the following review helpful.
Neo-Lysenkoism for dummies
By Jack
"Sophisticated people sneer at feel-good comedies and saccharine romances in which all loose ends are tied and everyone lives happily ever after. Life is nothing like that, we note, and we look to the arts for edification about the painful dilemmas of the human condition. Yet when it comes to the science of human beings, this same audience says: Give us schmaltz!"-Steven Pinker

1. MISUNDERSTANDING HERITABILITY

In the first chapter of The Genius in All of Us David Shenk writes that Charles Murray and Richard Herrnstein (henceforth, M&H), the authors of The Bell Curve, "fundamentally misinterpreted a number of studies, becoming convinced that roughly 60 percent of each person's intelligence comes directly from his or her genes." For at least two reasons, this claim is not a very auspicious start for a book that purports to challenge the vast research literature showing that genetic differences are important determinants of success. Firstly, no such claim is made anywhere in The Bell Curve. On the contrary, M&H write (p. 106) that "heritability describes something about a population of people, not an individual. It makes no more sense to talk about the heritability of an individual's IQ than it does to talk about his birthrate." Secondly, the fact that Shenk nevertheless thinks that M&H's thesis is that "60 percent of each person's intelligence comes directly from genes" indicates that Shenk does not understand what it means to say, as M&H do, that the HERITABILITY of a trait is such and such.

Heritability is a measure of the extent that DIFFERENCES among individuals have genetic causes, not a measure of the extent that genes have contributed to any individual's ontogenic development. For example, the heritability of height is at least 80 percent in first world countries, but that does NOT mean that 80 percent of any person's (or the average person's, as Shenk appears to interpret it) height is due to genes and the remaining 20 percent due to the environment. Heritability is a population statistic indicating the causes of VARIATION between individuals. Shenk doesn't understand this, and his discussion of genes and environments throughout the book is seriously handicapped by this fundamental mistake. Unfortunately, this is far from being the only instance of conceptual misunderstanding and misrepresentation in the book.

2. OVERSTATING GENOTYPE-ENVIRONMENT INTERACTION

Shenk continues by claiming that M&H are mistaken because they are unaware of genotype-environment interactions (GxE for short). This assertion would undoubtedly have surprised the late Richard Herrnstein who wrote about GxE at least as far back as in his 1973 book I.Q. in the Meritocracy, but let's leave that aside for the moment. Shenk writes that there's recently been a revolution in genetics: scientists used to think that genes cause phenotypes deterministically and in exactly the same way across all environments, but now they have realized that phenotypes in fact develop through complex interactions between genes and environments, or GxE. This account of the history of genetics is, of course, farcical, and bears no resemblance to reality. There's been no such revolution, and geneticists (as well as the general public) have always understood that interactions between genes and environment are crucial for the development of phenotypes. I'll write more about Shenk's bizarrely distorted view of the history of genetics later in this review, but I'll give one illustrative example here. He cites a 1957 study by Stanford anatomist William Walter Greulich which found that Japanese Americans grew up to be considerably taller than their cousins in the old country. Shenk suggests that Greulich was mystified by this result, but if you read the actual study, you'll see that Greulich sensibly attributed the difference to better nutrition in America. Astonishingly, Shenk appears to think that just about everybody was unaware of the link between malnutrition and stunted growth even as recently as the 1990s.

Over the last century, there have always been people who have maintained that it's a mistake to speak of nature and nurture as independent forces or to wonder whether it's genes or the environment that is more important in determining human differences. Joining this long line of people who don't understand quantitative and behavioral genetics, Shenk asserts that both genes and environment are needed and that it's wrong to speak of them as separate sources of influence. This is, of course, true and self-evident in a certain sense: an organism cannot exist if it doesn't have both a genome and an environment. Moreover, some individuals are, inevitably, born and raised in more favorable environments than others. However, this sort of GxE interaction poses no problems for quantifying the independent effects of genes and environment because heritability is not estimated at the level of an individual but rather at the population level. If you look at a single individual, trying to disentangle the effects of the environment and genes in producing a particular phenotype is indeed often a hopeless task. That's why quantitative geneticists step back from a single individual and instead look at a representative sample of individuals, asking whether similarities and differences in genotypes and environments correlate with phenotypic similarity among them. In animal and plant research, environments and genotypes can be directly manipulated (by using inbred strains and controlled environments, for example) in order to estimate heritability whereas in human research natural experiments (such as twinning and adoption) are utilized.

M&H noted in The Bell Curve that the more equal the environmental circumstances in America get, the more strongly differences in intelligence will be determined by genes. Shenk writes that they're mistaken and don't understand how genes work. Here as in many other places in the book, one wonders at the size of David Shenk's ego. He thinks that until recently, almost nobody doing research on these topics realized any of the simple ideas he presents in his book. Had he actually bothered to familiarize himself with the research on which M&H draw, he would have understood that the claim of greater importance for genes in more equal environments is a truism. It follows from the fact that at the population level genetic and environmental sources of variation are complements: when one decreases, the other increases, and vice versa. Thus if (trait-relevant) environmental variation decreases in society, then genetic variation will, by mathematical necessity, determine a larger share of phenotypic variation.

So, it's trivially true that both genes and environments are needed, and that some environments are better for everyone than others. GxE in this sense does not pose great problems for behavioral genetic analysis. However, there is a special sense in which GxE may complicate heritability estimation. This is when the same environment affects different genotypes differently, or, to put it another way, if the sensitivity of different genotypes to the same environment varies. This is usually referred to as 'statistical GxE' to differentiate it from the common, trivial GxE discussed above. Statistical GxE concerns such questions as whether genotype G1 is superior to genotype G2 universally, that is, across all known environments, or whether there are environments where G2 is less inferior, equal or, especially, superior to G1. For example, it could be that in environment E1 individuals with G1 will have a higher phenotypic value P than those with G2, whereas in environment E2 the rank order of individuals on P will be reversed. If strong GxE interaction effects in the statistical sense are present, then it will indeed be impossible to give a meaningful answer to the question of the relative importance of genes and the environment, and heritability estimates may become biased (depending on the research design).

The book discusses a famous example of statistical GxE, the Cooper and Zubek rat study. This old study from the 1950s is often cited because there are very few, if any, other comparable examples of statistical GxE in the literature. Cooper and Zubek had two inbred strains of rats, one of them "maze-bright", the other "maze-dull." They found that the bright rats beat the dull ones in a maze task when both were reared in a normal environment, but the two strains performed similarly when both were reared in either restricted or enriched environments. Shenk interprets this study as a seminal confirmation of GxE. However, in their paper Cooper and Zubek warn against making strong interpretations of the results until the study is replicated. Among other things, they note that the maze task may have been too easy to differentiate between the two strains in the enriched condition; if one of the strains was actually brighter, it could not have been detected. Tellingly, Cooper and Zubek's results have never been reproduced.

The fact that it's theoretically possible that statistical GxE is an important source of individual differences does not mean that it actually is. Whether statistical GxE effects exist for any particular trait is an empirical question. Behavioral genetic research on a wide range of cognitive, personality, and other human traits indicates that statistical GxE is at best a trivial source of variation, accounting for at most a few percent of population variation, and that it therefore cannot bias heritability estimates. For example, twin and adoption studies have demonstrated that statistical GxE effects for IQ are either very small or non-existent. However, the most powerful and direct refutation of the idea that statistical GxE interactions bias estimates of IQ heritability has come from recently developed DNA-based methods of heritability estimation. These new studies show incontrovertibly that the contribution of additive genetic effects to IQ variation is around 50 percent (and this figure is certainly an underestimate of total heritability because the method ignores the effects of rare genetic variants, dominance, and epistasis). The heritability of IQ in childhood is about 40 percent, and it increases linearly with age, reaching about 80 percent in adulthood. These estimates are not biased by any statistical GxE effects. (While it is less relevant for the sorts of phenotypes Shenk's book deals with, there is one domain of human behavior where some evidence exists for substantial statistical GxE effects. A handful of studies have indicated that there are mental and behavioral disorders, e.g., depression, whose occurrence may, to some extent, be dependent on interactions between vulnerable genotypes and bad environments.)

Characteristically, Shenk does not realize that the term GxE is (admittedly confusingly) used in the genetics literature in these two different, "common" and "statistical", technical senses and that the implications of the two are very different. After discussing the rat study, he lists other examples of GxE, such as how temperature around eggs determines the sex of turtles and snakes, or how the color of a grasshopper's skin is influenced by the environment. However, these other examples are just ordinary environmental effects that have nothing to do with statistical GxE. For Shenk, GxE is a buzzword that needs not be precisely defined and can mean just about anything. At times the book's claims about GxE are so expansive that it appears, as one commentator put it, that Shenk would be genuinely surprised if his own children looked anything like him or had any mental traits in common with him.

3. FUNHOUSE VERSION OF THE HISTORY OF GENETICS

The most curious thing about Shenk's book is how he has managed to get the history of genetics so wrong. For example, in his discussion of several works published in the 1990s, he claims that the authors attribute such a large role to genes in behavioral differences only because in the 1990s no one knew about interactions between genes and environment. However, the idea of GxE interaction (in the non-statistical sense) was formally introduced by the German zoologist Richard Woltereck in 1909. He called it the norm of reaction, and it has long been covered in standard textbooks. The absurdity of the claim that GxE in this sense was unknown in the 1990s becomes apparent when one considers that it would suggest that in the 1990s scientists, not to mention the general public, did not understand that, say, crop yields could be improved with fertilizers and pesticides.

The first investigation into GxE in the statistical sense was R.A. Fisher's 1923 experimental study on the effects of different manurial treatments on several varieties of potato. He concluded that there was no statistical GxE (he called it "non-linear interaction of environment and heredity"). Fisher always maintained that statistical GxE effects were unimportant because they were so rare. The case for the importance of statistical GxE was first made by the British zoologist Lancelot Hogben in the 1930s. He drew particularly on a study of fruit flies that showed evidence of (weak) statistical GxE. In contrast to Fisher, Hogben conjectured that statistical GxE effects are widespread in nature, making it an important third source of population variation besides genes and the environment. Later scientific disputes about GxE, such as the IQ debate of the 1970s in which Richard Lewontin, David Layzer, and others were pitted against Arthur Jensen, were essentially reprisals of the original Hogben-Fisher debate, with little more empirical evidence to back up the notion that statistical GxE could be important for human behavioral differences. As noted above, research since the 1930s has amply vindicated Fisher's skepticism about the practical significance of statistical GxE. Books about how everybody can be smarter and better than the average usually draw heavily on the arguments of Lewontin et co., but Shenk's doesn't because he seems to be almost completely oblivious to this long intellectual history, laboring as he is under the misconception that GxE was discovered in the 1990s or so.

Far from being some new revolutionary idea, GxE has been in the conceptual toolbox of geneticists ever since the beginnings of the science. Some vulgar Mendelians of the early 20th century may have had ideas of strictly deterministic genes, but Shenk's argument that this was the reigning orthodoxy in behavioral and developmental genetics until recently is utterly absurd.

4. THE FALLACY OF "DELIBERATE PRACTICE"

Shenk is infatuated with Anders Ericsson's theory of "deliberate practice", according to which the amount and quality of practice determines who will or will not excel in any given field, and innate differences have almost nothing to do with it. Shenk writes that the trick to becoming an expert or a top performer is to be extraordinarily motivated, unfazed by disappointment and lack of progress, and able to delay instant gratification. But this is simply a description of what high achievers are like. It says nothing about WHY they are like that. Just giving a name like deliberate practice to something that high achievers do doesn't explain anything. To think it does is the nominal fallacy.

Shenk writes that not only is it possible to become a high achiever in any field by practicing for 10,000 hours, but also that it is not possible to do it in less time. He cites research on chess players by Guillermo Campitelli and Fernand Gobet who indeed found that titled chess masters needed an average of about 10,000 hours to reach that level. However, note the word 'average' there. Shenk has fallen for the ecological fallacy, that is, making inferences about individuals based on the aggregate properties of the group to which they belong. Campitelli and Gobet have also found that there's a lot of individual variation in how much practice is needed to achieve master level in chess. Some players reach that level in as few as 3,000 hours of practice, whereas others need much more than 10,000 hours. Still others fail to reach master level even after 30,000 hours. The same pattern holds everywhere, not just in chess: some learn things much easier and much quicker than others.

Shenk cites an article by Anders Ericsson published in The International Journal of Sport Psychology where it is claimed that with the exception of body size, genetic differences do not contribute to elite sports achievement. He doesn't cite the article by Klissouras and colleagues, published in the same issue of the same journal (Vol. 38, Issue 1), who report, drawing on twin studies, that "individual differences in most functional abilities, morphological characteristics, motor attributes, personality and cognitive traits linked to superior sport performance are substantially influenced by genetic factors" and that "genetic influence is so ubiquitous and persuasive that we ask not what is heritable but what is not heritable." Stubbornly ignoring such confounding genetic variables indicates that one is only interested in promoting an agenda.

The book's account of the development of excellence is purely descriptive, ending where it should properly begin. Why is it that some people have the energy and the will to practice day in and day out, year after year? Are there ways to becoming more conscientious and motivated? Why do some people reach a top level of performance so much faster that others who work equally hard? Why are there large differences among top performers in any field, independent of the amount of practice? Most of these questions are not even raised in the book.

One of the most replicated and most interesting findings from behavioral genetics is that for IQ, personality, and many other complex traits, heritability increases with age, while the effect of the childhood environment pretty much completely fades away by adulthood. It is believed that this is because genetic differences express themselves only gradually during development, and/or because growing up, people increasingly select environments that better match their natural inclinations, resulting in positive or negative feedback loops. This would tie nicely into Shenk's discussion of how child prodigies often fail to grow up to be high achievers in adulthood, but ignorant as he is of (and hostile to) behavioral genetics, this connection is never made in the book. In general, it's of course expedient for Shenk to ignore the fact that genetic factors become more, not less, important with age, as this finding is difficult to fit together with his overarching interactionist argument.

Charles Murray, who serves as an archetype of the wicked hereditarian in Shenk's book, has argued that the smart and successful are so mainly due to their luck in the genetic lottery, and should therefore be educated in the spirit of noblesse oblige. In contrast, Shenk maintains that genes have little or nothing to do with success, and that anyone who has failed to succeed has only themselves to blame; the successful are simply morally superior. I think Murray's view not only makes much more sense but is also more humane than Shenk's. Where does Shenk think that the personal qualities that he says lead to excellence -- such as perseverance and zeal -- come from? He seems to believe that people freely choose what their personality is like. This presupposes an extent of control over one's own behavior that people simply don't have.

5. UNDERESTIMATING THE POWER OF IQ

The measurement of intelligence is perhaps the greatest success story of psychology. IQ tests are highly reliable instruments measuring a stable human characteristic which has a predictive power stronger and more universal than that of any other social science variable. Shenk grudgingly admits as much, but then argues incoherently that it does not matter and that IQ is in fact highly malleable. The stability of IQ is an empirical fact. To take an extreme example, in a unique study by Ian Deary and colleagues, the correlation between IQ measured at ages 11 and 80 in a Scottish sample was 0.63. With statistical corrections for attenuation and range restriction, the correlation was as high as 0.80, which represents a truly remarkable level of stability over 69 years, especially as IQ is not yet fully developed at age 11 and by age 80 many people suffer from neurodegenerative diseases.

Here, as elsewhere, Shenk is hobbled by his inability to conceptualize things at different levels of analysis. At the level of a particular individual, IQ may indeed show substantial instability, particularly in childhood. The reasons for this are many and include, for example, individual differences in childhood developmental trajectories, accidents of test situation, illnesses and injuries, training effects, and random measurement error in general. However, most people do not experience large score discrepancies between tests, which ensures stability at the population level.

Shenk claims that the large shifts in IQ that some individuals experience mean that we know how to change IQs. This is not correct. Despite much effort, we still do not know how to produce intelligence gains of significant magnitude. We know that test scores may be increased by suitable training, but such coaching does not make people smarter and over time these hollow gains fade away completely. We know that while genetic factors explain the majority of IQ differences, environmental effects play a role, too, but what those effects might be remains a mystery.

The book discusses Hart and Risley's oft-cited study where it was found that children whose parents talk more to them tend to have higher IQs. Shenk is convinced that this correlation represents a causal effect. He quotes the American Psychological Association's 1996 report on intelligence research where it was noted that the results of Hart and Risley's study are causally ambiguous because such correlations may be confounded by genetic effects. Shenk contemptuously dismisses this argument, and asserts that the correlation must be causal because of GxE. It seems that for him, GxE is a magic word that can miraculously convert any correlation into a causal effect, without any need for evidence of causality. The APA report correctly notes that causal interpretations of correlations between IQ and the family environment are difficult to reconcile with evidence from twin and adoption studies. Research shows that after early childhood the shared family environment has a negligible influence on IQ differences, outside of extremes of environmental deprivation. What renders any hypothesis about strong family effects on IQ prima facie implausible is the fact that the IQs of siblings from the same family vary widely, and that the IQs of unrelated people raised together are not more similar than those of two randomly selected people from the same population.

The book also describes Robert Sternberg's "triarchic" intelligence theory. Sternberg's major claim is that IQ tests measure only "academic intelligence", whereas real-life success is dependent on "practical intelligence." In support of this theory, Sternberg has presented evidence showing that, for example, highly experienced manual workers are superior at the sort of mental calculations needed in their jobs than higher-IQ but inexperienced white-collar workers. Shenk asserts that these sort of findings "pose a serious challenge to research psychologists adhering to traditional definitions of intelligence." Of course, he doesn't actually discuss what such research psychologists think about Sternberg's theory.

Perhaps the most thorough refutation of Sternberg's theory was published in issue 4, 2003, of the academic journal Intelligence. In their articles, Linda Gottfredson and Nathan Brody showed that all of Sternberg's findings are explainable in terms of traditional psychometric intelligence theory. They note, for example, that Sternberg's examples of "practical intelligence" represent narrow skills honed through lots of practice. Research has established that general intelligence is very difficult to train, but narrow, task-specific skills and abilities are of course trainable. Shenk himself gives a good example of this in his discussion of the experiment where a man was able to greatly improve his memory for sequences of numbers through intensive practice, but this did not improve his other skills at all, not even the ability to remember sequences of letters. The claim of IQ, or general mental ability, to being a valid operationalization of intelligence rests precisely on the fact that it is not a narrow trained skill but a highly general, context-independent capacity. Higher IQ makes it easier to solve novel problems and to acquire every kind of knowledge and expertise. As a result, those with higher IQs tend to develop a broader and deeper expertise and have careers commensurate with that, with no need to learn skills required in some particular low-skilled job. Sternberg claims that the greater the overlap in the specific skills measured by a test and those needed in a job is, the greater is the test's predictive validity for that job. In fact, the contrary is true: tests of general mental ability with no job-specific content are clearly superior predictors of job performance than tests assessing specific job-related skills.

In his reanalysis of Sternberg's data, Brody dealt a coup de gr�ce to Sternberg's research program by showing that the intelligence tests Sternberg had developed in order to measure three supposedly independent domains of intelligence were in fact correlated with each other. Damningly, the predictive validity of the tests was entirely a function of the extent that they measured general mental ability.

Shenk asserts that "[i]ntelligence scores of infants are not predictive of future scores or life success." This is a key claim in his critique of IQ, but alas, it's not true. For example, Joseph Fagan and colleagues developed a test of visual memory (which is a pretty good proxy for general mental ability at any age) and then showed in a longitudinal study that the disattenuated correlations of six-to-twelve-month old infants' visual memory skills with adult IQ and academic achievement were 0.59 and 0.53, respectively ("The prediction, from infancy, of adult IQ and achievement." Intelligence, Volume 35, Issue 3, May-June 2007, Pages 225-231). Thus infant test scores do predict adult IQ and achievement. Cognitive inequality is evident already in the cradle.

The book's treatment of the Flynn effect, the observed increase in mean IQ scores across generations, is predictably misinformed. The book appears to argue that the Flynn effect represents an actual increase in intelligence, but statistical analyses show that measurement invariance does not hold in between-generation IQ comparisons. This means that IQ tests do not give unbiased estimates of the IQs of older cohorts when compared to those of younger cohorts. This is somewhat similar to giving an English-language IQ test to a non-native speaker: the obtained IQ score will be downwardly biased and will not reflect the true level of ability. The past is a foreign country in the case of IQ, too. It may be that people have indeed got smarter over the last few generations -- just as the average height has increased -- but this cannot be established based on simple IQ comparisons.

Shenk makes much of the fact that relatively few of the participants in Lewis Terman's classic study of intellectually precocious children made significant creative achievements as adults. He concludes that IQ is thus not associated with creativity. However, as Murray and Herrnstein demonstrated in The Bell Curve, the social and economic value of high cognitive ability increased enormously during the 20th century. Terman's "Termites" were born around 1910, and in those days it was normal for highly intelligent individuals to end up in unremarkable jobs. In the second half of the 20th century the intellectual sorting of American society rapidly intensified as universities and employers started seeking out high ability individuals all over the country. The results of this change can be seen in the Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth, or the SMPY, an ongoing longitudinal study similar to Terman's but started in 1971 (despite the name, it is not limited only to the mathematically gifted). The results of the SMPY are quite different from Terman's study. What the SMPY has established is that intelligence test scores measured in childhood are robust predictors of later creative accomplishments, including literary works and scientific articles published, PhDs and patents earned, and attainment of tenure-track positions at top research universities.

6. CONFIRMATION BIAS RUN AMUCK

Throughout the book, Shenk's discussion of relevant literature is highly selective and one-sided. He attempts to defend himself by saying that he has a particular viewpoint that he wants to promote and that he relies on those studies and researchers he has found to be the most credible. While I don't mind books making a bold case for something, I think it's reasonable to expect a partisan author to engage the evidence and arguments of the other side and try to refute them. Shenk makes no such attempt. He has not talked to those with views different from his, nor familiarized himself with their work. He misunderstands basic concepts and ideas, constructing straw man versions of opposing arguments to be knocked down. He has simply followed his confirmation bias by seeking out people and studies that lend support to the views he had from the outset. He accepts even the most implausible claims of his favorite authors, while ignoring other views and contrary evidence.

One example is his criticism of the Minnesota Twin Study which found that identical twins separated in infancy grew up to be remarkably similar in numerous ways. He tries to dismiss these results by bringing up the well-worn criticism that many of the twin pairs had been reunited long before the study, arguing that this, rather than shared genes, was what explained the similarities. What he doesn't mention is that the authors of the Minnesota study have responded to this criticism by analyzing whether those twins who had known each other longer were more similar than those who had only recently met. The answer was negative, refuting the criticism. Shenk's criticisms of twin studies are uninformed throughout, and he does not seem to even realize that the results of twin studies have been corroborated by other research designs, such as those based on adoptees and extended kinships.

Shenk repeatedly acclaims that genes are not meaningless, that he's not claiming that humans are blank slates. However, this is mere obfuscation, because the entire book is an attack on the idea that genes are a significant cause of human differences. Shenk's neo-Lysenkoism reaches a crescendo in the last chapter dealing with epigenetics. He claims, among other things, that you can make your future kids innately smarter by being intellectually active. Needless to say, there is no evidence for such an absurd suggestion. In recent decades, cumulating behavioral genetic evidence has dismantled the intellectual edifice of anti-hereditarian thinking. Preposterous, unsupported claims made about epigenetic effects are the last, desperate refuge of this school of pseudo-science.

To sum up, Shenk is hopelessly confused about several key concepts; is astonishingly ignorant of the history of the nature/nurture debate; ignores or misrepresents research that contradicts his claims; places enormous trust on anecdotes and the results of never-replicated experiments; ignores entire research traditions and the best, methodologically most sophisticated studies; and thinks that if he gives a name to a phenomenon he has explained it (i.e., the nominal fallacy). The book is permeated by relentless optimism about human potential but the ideas in it do not support the optimism. Shenk's toxic combination of arrogance and ignorance and the fact that many people have bought into his brand of snake oil make me a bit more pessimistic about humans.

16 of 18 people found the following review helpful.
An effective deconstruction of hereditary talent, and clues for a new model of exceptional ability
By Todd I. Stark
It is easy to like or dislike this book from a casual reading based on how you feel about the premise: that everyone has the potential for genius, and that heredity is not destiny in any sense.
This sounds at first like a liberal political statement, but Shenk's treatment is far more nuanced than that characterization would imply.

In brief, Shenk's book is a very good deconstruction of hereditary talent, a competent but one-sided (or upon reflection I'll say very selectively focused) review of supporting research in several fields, and an interesting but abbreviated practical introduction to the interactionist (gene X environment) paradigm of development.

Just to be clear, this book is not about the psychometric definition of genius in terms of how far down the bell curve one is on Raven's Progressive Matrices or standardized tests of any sort. Nor is it about clever calculating tricks or precocious abilities, although it does do a very nice job putting those into a larger perspective. This book is more centrally about the expansive and inclusive sense of genius meaning people that accomplish something truly special and significant, and the potential that any given person may be able to get to that point. Somehow. And that's where the nuance is needed and appropriate.

Ok, I didn't like this book all that much when I first read it, and I at first gave it a mediocre 3 star rating on Amazon. I felt it did a great job deconstructing the concept of hereditary talent, but I strongly criticized it for leaving a gap where we need a better theory of where talent comes from and what it is, since obviously we don't all become true geniuses. Even among the folks who appear to have the seeds of genius in them from early on, most don't become genius adults in the broader sense.

In my original review I said this was a one-sided review of the evidence for the interactionist model. I do think it's a very selective review, but one-sided implies that he deliberately ignores contradictory evidence. He doesn't do that. He just doesn't talk about the evidence that led to the model Shenk says is obsolete, that genes are akin to blueprints. That is, the evidence that different variations of allele sometimes have strikingly specific effects in a seemingly "normal" range of environments. The case for the model of heredity that Shenk is deconstructing is not entirely ignored, but it is glossed over in order to make his case for the interactionist model. I think that is why hereditarians like Galton, Spearman, and Charles Murray get so apparently shorted in this book, Shenk focuses entirely on what they get wrong and glosses over the things they may get right. I suspect that we do inherit "predispositions" in some form under a very wide range of conditions, even if the underlying mechanism is more complex than we previously assumed. Even if changes in environments do alter the expression of genes, something like inheritance of traits clearly does happen in a wide range of "normal" environments, and we can't just ignore that completely because of additional complexity and things that change at the extremes. That's why I say this is a very selective review. But no, it isn't really one-sided, the selectiveness is appropriate for a deconstruction, although it does mark this as a deconstruction rather than a scholarly review.

The more important problem is that the model of talent that arises from this book is not particularly easy to understand. The author is strongly against thinking of genes as predispositions, and rather offers the perspective that genes are akin to "settings." So it would be easy to conclude that the author is saying that we have the ability to make anyone a genius just by tweaking a few settings. He isn't. Or, if you read it as I did upon my first reading, you might hear the author saying that "anyone can be a genius, but talent is complicated process, we don't know what is happening at each step, and so we don't know how to help people get there, but we know it's possible." That's perhaps a little closer to the truth, but it didn't seem very helpful to me.

The reason I updated this review and why I'm now expressing more appreciation for David Shenk's accomplishment here is that while the "settings" model of genes doesn't quite convey the message, I did find upon close reading and careful reflection that the author captured a lot with his examples and case studies of individuals. The thing that is missing is some way of tying together how people manage to select and shape environments for themselves to accomplish great things, in spite of all the cultural, social, and physical constraints that tend to make environmental factors very hard to change for most of us. Shenk assiduously avoids attributing "predispositions" to genes, but then speculates that epigenetic factors may predispose us to things like musical ability. If non-genes can do this, why not genes? He just seems a little *too* intent on crushing hereditary talent in some places.

Geniuses don't just see things differently (although that is sometimes also going on), they don't just have unique abilities (although sometimes they do) geniuses are most distinct in that they manage to carve their own niche, exploiting their own uniqueness in a process where they are driven to mastery and are amazingly persistent, even where the goal seems way out of reach. This runs contrary to our popular wisdom that it makes sense to work toward small easily attained goals in most things. What we think of as really deep talent actually requires really deep faith in the long term process and the motivation to keep going. Shenk captures the significance of motivation, but I had to look very closely to see the patterns for it. It requires willingness to do things that others may find bizarre and to learn freely from what is available. The author illustrates this but seems to have a hard time really tying it all together, at least he did on my first reading. I've come to think of it in terms of niche construction, which to me really captures what exceptional people do that brings out and shapes their unique gene x environment combination in a targeted way. My reversal in the rating reflects my feeling that capturing this idea is more important than giving it a catchy name, which is really what the author is missing.

We don't know exactly how to take advantage of the dynamic nature of heredity and development, although the study of achievement and expertise reviewed by Shenk gives us many tantalizing clues to go on. And if knowing that the potential is there inspires the faith to keep going, then more and more of us will eventually learn to become better and better at using our minds, constructing our own niches from our own individuality, and the promise of "The Genius in All of Us" will eventually begin to be realized. There is a lot in this book that will repay careful reading and re-reading, as I discovered by doing exactly that.

Related Reading:

See also this classic manifesto of genetic interactionism: The Triple Helix: Gene, Organism, and Environment(Lewontin R (1998/2000) Triple Helix: Gene, Organism, Environment. Cambridge, MA, Harvard)

This superb earlier popular introduction to the emerging model Shenk offers: The Agile Gene: How Nature Turns on Nurture(Ridley, The Agile Gene)

This similar treatment of trait development in interactionist terms, but focused on personality: The Temperamental Thread: How Genes, Culture, Time and Luck make Us Who We Are(kagan, temperamental thread)

This alternative and original interactionist account of how personality develops: No Two Alike: Human Nature and Human Individuality (Judith rich harris no two alike)

This interesting challenge to some widely help assumptions about influence: Stranger in the Nest: Do Parents Really Shape Their Child's Personality, Intelligence, or Character?(Stranger in the Nest, D. Cohen)

This little known treasure by an old friend that offers its own unique challenges about human uniqueness and what it means: rebellion: physics to personal will (Brody, Rebellion)

This psychometric study of what distinguishes geniuses in terms of personality: Genius: The Natural History of Creativity (Problems in the Behavioural Sciences)

This on the classic view from the perspective of behavior genetics: Genetics and Experience: The Interplay between Nature and Nurture (Individual Differences and Development)(Plomin, Genetics and Experience)

This on the fascinating broader biological implications of interactionism from a gene perspective, how the genes of organisms construct niches even beyond the organism itself: The Extended Organism: The Physiology of Animal-Built Structures(Turner, The Extended Organism)

And finally this wonderful broad account of biology and the role of heredity that appreciates the complexities of gene function in a demanding but uniquely engaging way: The Logic of Life(The logic of life, francois jacob)

See all 94 customer reviews...

The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk PDF
The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk EPub
The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk Doc
The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk iBooks
The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk rtf
The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk Mobipocket
The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk Kindle

The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk PDF

The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk PDF

The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk PDF
The Genius in All of Us: New Insights into Genetics, Talent, and IQ, by David Shenk PDF